Jeffress model

From Scholarpedia
Peter Cariani (2011), Scholarpedia, 6(7):2920. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.2920 revision #137337 [link to/cite this article]
Jump to: navigation, search
Post-publication activity

Curator: Peter Cariani

Figure 1: The tapped delay lines, coincidence detectors and coincidence counters (rate integrators) of the Jeffress model.

The Jeffress model is a neurocomputational model that explains how auditory systems can register and analyze small differences in the arrival time of sounds at the two ears in order to estimate the direction of sound sources in the azimuthal plane.

Contents

History

In 1948, Lloyd Jeffress sought to outline “a structural mechanism for representing a time difference spatially” (Jeffress, 1948). Jeffress explicitly had E.G. Boring’s general neural place-theory hypothesis in mind: that “all knowledge [conscious distinction] is potentially spatial in the physiological sense” and that “we are looking for a place theory of every dimension” (from Boring (1933) as quoted in Jeffress (1948), brackets by the author of this article).

Jeffress's proposed time-delay neural network model receives two sets of stimulus-locked spike train signals from the left and right auditory pathways and uses a set of delay lines and coincidence detectors to compute a temporal cross-correlation function. Due to the systematic spatial arrangement of the delay lines and coincidence detectors, the network converts an interaural temporal disparity into a maximum place of neural excitation. The network was perhaps the first explicit neural information-processing model to successfully account for major aspects of perceptual function (see note 1). Although many of its details have been revised over the last half century, the Jeffress architecture, with its temporally coded input signals, delay lines, and coincidence detectors remains at the core of current conceptualizations of how humans and animals perform binaural localization using interaural time difference cues.

Psychophysics

The model was formulated to account for binaural psychophysical observations. Humans are capable of using interaural time-of-arrival differences (ITDs) of as small as 10-20 microseconds to distinguish directional differences of sound sources in the horizontal plane as small as 1-2 degrees (azimuth). Typically ITDs range from zero for sounds coming from directly in front to about 700 microseconds for sounds coming directly from either side. (See Durlach and Colburn (1978) and Colburn and Durlach (1978) for comprehensive reviews of binaural psychophysics and models thereof.)

Time-delay neural network

The time-delay neural architecture ( Figure 1) consists of

  1. temporally-coded input signals consisting of spikes that are time-locked to the waveform of the acoustic stimulus,
  2. two sets of tapped conduction delay lines that differentially delay these monaural neural patterns that are then fed into
  3. an array of binaural spike coincidence detectors, whose outputs are then inputs into
  4. coincidence counters that provide the number of coincidences as a function of relative delay.

The delay lines are arranged in antiparallel fashion to implement a range of available relative delays between the two sets of monaural lines. These relative delays then are systematically mapped to particular spatial places within the coincidence array. The axonal conduction lines make excitatory synapses onto the coincidence detector elements. When incoming pulses arrive at roughly the same time at a given detector, within a given temporal coincidence window, the detector emits an output pulse. Trains of output pulses are then transmitted to coincidence counters, which sum spikes over a given temporal integration window to compute the running firing rates of their respective coincidence detectors. In the 1948 paper Jeffress did not explicitly include coincidence counters or discharge rate integrators, but instead talks in terms of more or less excitation (higher firing rate) in one or another part of the delay array. For example, a sound source directly in front of the listener would maximally excite coincidence detectors in the middle of the array (with zero relative delay), whilst a highly lateralized source at the sides would excite detectors at one edge of the array. See also Albeck (1995) for a succinct neurocomputational overview.

Jeffress envisaged that each cochlear place or frequency channel would map to a separate neural cross-correlation circuit, such that the whole architecture consists of an array of these circuits ( Figure 2).

The inputs to the binaural time-delay cross-correlator come from the left and right auditory neural pathways. In mammalian auditory systems, incoming acoustic signals are first mechanically filtered by the outer, middle, and inner ear (cochlea) and transduced by inner hair cells of the cochlea into synaptic currents that in turn produce action potentials in auditory nerve fibers. Each auditory fiber innervates one cochlear inner hair cell and projects in a cochleotopic manner to a small number of spherical bushy cells in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus. Their large, highly secure synapses, called endbulbs of Held, drive the target spherical cells to produce trains of action potentials that preserve or even strengthen the precise spike timing of the auditory nerve (cf., Joris et al., 1994). The spherical cells, in turn, project to bipolar cells in both the left and right brainstem, specifically the medial superior olivary nuclei (MSO), again preserving the cochleotopic organization of the mappings. (See Casseday and Covey (1987) for an overview of the neuroanatomy.)

Figure 2: Inputs to the left binaural cross correlator.

Although the rough outlines of major ascending auditory pathways and the existence of phase-locked responses in the auditory nerve were known in 1948, the circuitry in the auditory brainstem that might support binaural crosscorrelation had not been worked out. Jeffress considered both the lateral superior olivary complex and the lateral lemniscus as possible loci, but correctly rejected them on physiological grounds. Jeffress was then left with situating his coincidence detectors either in the auditory midbrain (inferior colliculus) or thalamus (medial geniculate body) see note 2. It should be noted that Jeffress later (1958) rejected these more central nuclei in favor of the MSO.

The architecture computes a binaural crosscorrelation function for each frequency region of the cochlea ( Figure 3), resulting in two (left and right) symmetrically reflected frequency-ITD rate-place maps. Thus a pattern of temporal disparities is mapped onto a spatial pattern of excitation (place principle). See Colburn (1996) and Trahiotis et al. (2006) for discussion of the implications of this frequency-interaural time delay (ITD) representation for explaining diverse aspects of binaural psychophysics.

The model has been implemented in silico using analog very large scale integrated (VLSI) circuits (Lazzaro et al. 1991; Lazzaro and Mead 1989, 1990). Subsequent neuroanatomy and neurophysiology has largely confirmed the basic premises of the basic model (see (Joris and Yin 2007; Joris 2006; Joris et al. 1998) albeit (arguably) with better correspondences in birds than in mammals. Areas of significant continuing debate are the distribution of the available internal delays and the mechanisms by which the delays are generated. Although Jeffress (1948) talked explicitly about “densities of coincidence cells” that varied with the delay (suggesting more delays near zero), the actual distribution of available delays and the dependence of that distribution on characteristic frequency is still an active topic. The role of the observed inhibitory inputs to the MSO neurons is also a topic of ongoing debate. (See Campbell and King, 2004; McAlpine, 2005; McAlpine and Grothe, 2003; Brand et al., 2002; Zhou et al., 2005.) Whether these finer discrepancies between the original Jeffress' model assumptions and the more recent neurophysiological data constitute a refutation of the core signal processing principles of the model (binaural temporal cross-correlation operations on temporally-coded inputs) has been a matter of ongoing discussion and debate.

Figure 3: The output representation of the Jeffress network. The output of the network is a spatial map of running coincidence rates, arranged by frequency channel (cochleotopic projection) and interaural delay.

The 1948 Jeffress model for binaural localization was closely followed in 1951 by J.C.R. Licklider’s duplex temporal autocorrelation model for pitch (Licklider 1951) in which monaural signals were delayed relative to a copy of themselves. As with the Jeffress model, each cochlear place (frequency channel) mapped into its own autocorrelation circuit. Thus, Licklider’s model was able to simultaneously produce both temporal and rate-place representations of pitch. In order to account for both monaural and binaural hearing, Licklider’s triplex model (Licklider 1959) combined a Jeffress-like crosscorrelation stage (sans rate integration) followed by an autocorrelation stage to form a generalized central correlation analyzer. Colin Cherry also proposed an architecture for monaural and binaural scene analysis (Cherry 1961) that utilized monaural autocorrelation analyses followed by binaural crosscorrelation operations and also included level weighting to allow readouts more generally related to lateral position.

In addition to binaural localization, the Jeffress model is potentially applicable to any sensory system in which 1) timing of spikes is correlated with the time structure of an external stimulus (stimulus-locked or phase-locked) and 2) there is a temporal disparity between the arrival times of a stimulus at different locations of receptor surfaces. In addition to auditory localization such systems include mechanoception, electroception, and vision (Carr 1993). In a series of striking experiments (many of them on himself), Georg von Bekesy showed how small (< 1 ms) temporal disparities in the stimulation of skin using delayed mechanical and electrical pulse pairs produce systematic changes in the perceived locus of the stimulus on the body surface (Bekesy von 1967). He carried out similar delayed pulse pair experiments using odorants puffed to the two nostrils (Bekesy von 1964a) and tastants spritzed to the two sides of the tongue (Bekesy von 1964b). Like auditory and mechanoceptive afferents, visual neurons also phase-lock to the temporal modulations that are produced at each retinotopic location when an image moves relative to a retina. Reichardt detectors compute temporal crosscorrelations between spikes produced when a visual image is successively presented to two visual receptor elements (ommatidia) at two different times (i.e. a moving image) (Reichardt 1961). An array of Reichardt motion detectors arranged to detect many different temporal delays across nearby retinal elements is therefore analogous (albeit with more dimensions) to a Jeffress coincidence architecture that detects delays across the two ears.

The Jeffress architecture is a classical example of a time-delay neural network that converts temporal disparities in the arrival of input signals into a rate-place frequency-ITD pattern. Valentino Braitenberg proposed a Jeffress-like architecture for his general-purpose cerebellar timing model (Braitenberg 1961; Braitenberg 1967; Braitenberg 2000) and delay-coincidence models for the cerebral cortex and hippocampus are also conceivable. Although the dominant assumption has been that time patterns are converted to rate-place patterns or rate-based channel-activation, other Jeffress-like cross-correlation networks can readily be envisioned that produce other kinds of output representations. For example, a neural cross-correlator can have as its output a spatiotemporal pattern of relative response latencies (if the different coincidence units have different conduction velocities that systematically amplify delay durations). The response latencies produced by the cross-correlator can then be compared with those of an unprocessed signal. In the binaural case, hypothetically, the longer the detour through the binaural cross-correlator relative to the ipsilateral direct monaural path might indicate a greater degree of lateralization on the ipsilateral side. If the temporal structure of the coincidence output spike trains are analyzed (rather than simply reporting firing rate), a general purpose cross-correlator can be used for a variety of temporal pattern recognition operations, e.g. as a temporal pattern sieve for extracting embedded spike patterns and for separating (demultiplexing) temporal pattern mixtures into their respective components (Cariani 2001).

Notes

1. The spatial shifter circuits proposed by Pitts and McCulloch in 1947 ("How we know universals: the perception of auditory and visual forms" Bull. Math. Biophys. 9 127-147) for musical transposition and visual magnification invariance have not passed the test of time. Also, it is entirely puzzling why the Jeffress and Licklider models have been largely omitted from the neurocomputational canon. (For example, neither model was presented in the two very influential and massive Neurocomputing volumes that were published by MIT Press during the field's inception in the late 1980s.)

2. It is possible that auditory scientists of the time could have considered the medial superior olivary nucleus as a possible (correct) candidate, since Lorente de No had carried out his pioneering neuroanatomical study of the auditory brainstem by the early 1930s. Unfortunately, Lorente’s work was not published until nearly 50 years later (Lorente, 1981) because of Depression-era budgets and the expense of publishing the many Golgi photomicrographs that constituted the primary evidence.

3. In his original 1948 paper Jeffress definitely considered the superior olive as a candidate locus because of its anatomical connections. He rejected this possibility based on evidence for phase-locking to the two ears and their "equal representation" at the level of the lateral lemniscus, which is above the superior olive. The finding in 1957/1958 of binaural interaction at the level of the lateral lemniscus and superior olive showed that Jeffress' original hunch regarding the superior olive was correct.

References

  • Albeck Y, 1995 "Sound localization and binaural processing", in The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks Ed M A Arbib (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge, MA) pp 891-895.
  • Bekesy von G, 1964a "Olfactory analogue to directional hearing" Journal of Applied Physiology 19, 369-373.
  • Bekesy von G, 1964b "Rythmical variations accompanying gustatory stimulation observed by means of localization phenomena" Journal of General Physiology 47, 809-825.
  • Bekesy von G, 1967 Sensory Inhibition (Princeton: Princeton University Press)
  • Boring E G, 1933 The Physical Dimensions of Consciousness (New York: Dover)
  • Braitenberg V, 1961 "Functional interpretation of cerebellar histology" Nature 190, 539-540.
  • Braitenberg V, 1967 "Is the cerebellar cortex a biological clock in the millisecond range?" Prog. Brain Res. 25, 334-346.
  • Braitenberg V, 2000 "The neuroanatomy of time", in Time and the Brain Ed R Miller (Australia: Australia) pp 391-396.
  • Brand A, Behrend O, Marquardt T, McAlpine D, Grothe B, 2002 “Precise inhibition is essential for microsecond interaural time difference coding,” Nature 417, 543-­547.
  • Campbell, Robert A. A. and Andrew J. King, 2004. "Auditory neuroscience: a time for coincidence?" Current Biology 14, R886-R888.
  • Cariani P, 2001 "Neural timing nets" Neural Networks 14, 737-753.
  • Carr C E, 1993 "Processing of temporal information in the brain" Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 223-243.
  • Casseday J H, Covey E, 1987 "Central auditory pathways in directional hearing", in Directional Hearing Eds W A Yost and G Gourevitch (New York: New York) pp 109-145.
  • Cherry C, 1961 "Two ears – but one world", in Sensory Communication Ed W A Rosenblith (New York: New York) pp 99-117.
  • Colburn H S, 1996 "Computational models of binaural processing", in Auditory Computation Eds H Hawkins, T McMullin, A N Popper and R R Fay (New York: New York)
  • Colburn H S, Durlach N I, 1978 "Models of binaural interaction", in Handbook of Perception Eds E C Carterette and M P Friedman (New York: New York) pp 467-518.
  • Durlach N I, Colburn H S, 1978 “Binaural phenomena,” in Handbook of Perception, Eds E C Carterette and M P Friedman (New York: New York) pp 365-466.
  • Jeffress L A, 1948 "A place theory of sound localization," J Comp Physiol Psychol 41, 35-39.
  • Jeffress L A, 1958 "Medial geniculate body - a disavowal," J Acoust Soc Am 30, 802-803.
  • Joris P X, Carney L H, Smith P H, Yin T C, (1994) “Enhancement of neural synchronization in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus. I. Responses to tones at the characteristic frequency,” J. Neurophysiol. 71, 1022-1036.
  • Joris P X, Yin T C, 2007 "A matter of time: internal delays in binaural processing" Trends Neurosci 30, 70-8.
  • Joris P X, 2006 "A dogged pursuit of coincidence" J Neurophysiol 96, 969-72.
  • Joris P X, Smith P H, Yin T C, 1998 "Coincidence detection in the auditory system: 50 years after Jeffress" Neuron 21, 1235-1238.
  • Lazzaro J, Arreguit X, Mead C, 1991 "Analog VLSI models of binaural hearing" IEEE Journal of Neural Networks 2, 230-236.
  • Lazzaro J, Mead C, 1989 "Silicon models of binaural localization" Neural Computation 1, 41-70.
  • Lazzaro J, Mead C, 1990 "Silicon models of auditory localization", in An Introduction to Neural and Electronic Networks Ed D Zornetzer, Lau (New York: New York) pp 158-174.
  • Licklider J C R, 1951 "A duplex theory of pitch perception" Experientia VII, 128-134.
  • Licklider J C R, 1959 "Three auditory theories", in Psychology: A Study of a Science. Study I. Conceptual and Systematic Ed S Koch (New York: New York) pp 41-144.
  • McAlpine D, 2005 "Creating a sense of auditory space" J Physiol 566, 21-28.
  • McAlpine D, Grothe B, 2003 "Sound localization and delay lines--do mammals fit the model?" Trends Neurosci 26, 347-350.
  • Pitts W, McCulloch W S, 1947 "How we know universals: the perception of auditory and visual forms" Bull. Math. Biophys. 9, 127-147.
  • Reichardt W, 1961 "Autocorrelation, a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system", in Sensory Communication Ed W A Rosenblith (New York: New York) pp 303-317.
  • Trahiotis, Constantine, Leslie R. Bernstein, Richard M. Stern, and Thomas N. Buell, 2006 "Interaural correlation as the basis of a working model of binaural processing: an introduction", in Popper, Arthur N. and Richard R. Fay, eds. 2006. Sound Source Localization. Springer, New York.
  • Zhou Y, Carney L H, Colburn H S, 2005 “A model for interaural time difference sensitivity in the medial superior olive: Interaction of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs, channel dynamics, and cellular morphology,” J. Neurosci. 25, 3046-3058.

Recommended reading

  • Albeck Y, 1995 "Sound localization and binaural processing", in The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks Ed M A Arbib (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge, MA) pp 891-895 [Compact synopsis of the Jeffress model from a neurocomputational perspective]
  • Colburn S, Durlach N I, 1978 "Models of binaural interaction", in Handbook of Perception Eds E C Carterette and M P Friedman (New York: New York) pp 467-518 [Large, comprehensive review of binaural psychophysics and models]
  • Joris P, Yin T C, 2007 "A matter of time: internal delays in binaural processing" Trends Neurosci 30 70-8. [Covers more recent controversies over time delays vs. inhibition.]
  • Popper, Arthur N. and Richard R. Fay, eds. 2006. Sound Source Localization. Springer, New York. [Contains many chapters on contemporary theory of spatial hearing.]

External links

See also

Auditory cortex, Cochlea

Personal tools
Namespaces

Variants
Actions
Navigation
Focal areas
Activity
Tools